Skip to main content

Florida Career College to Close

Florida Career College to Close Doug Lederman Fri, 01/26/2024 - 03:00 AM Byline(s) Doug Lederman from Inside Higher Ed https://ift.tt/avZRfLi

How you talk to your child might make them smarter

Excerpts of adult explanations from a working paper, “How does a switch work? The relation between adult mechanistic language and children’s learning,” by Katelyn Kurkul et al. Young children who were exposed to more mechanistic explanations learned more.

Why is it that higher income kids tend to score better on achievement tests than poor kids, even at the youngest ages? One explanation from the 1990s is the so-called 30-million word gap, in which researchers observed how higher income parents talked to their kids more and estimated that low income kids heard 30 million fewer words before the age of four.

But a new generation of researchers has been questioning whether the quantity of words really matters. Two studies in 2014 found that the quality of interaction between parents and babies was a better predictor of language skills.  A 2018 neuroscience study calculated that back-and-forth conversation was more important to brain development than the sheer number of words.

Now scholars are dissecting early childhood speech even more and analyzing the content of these back-and-forth conversations. Thanks to advances in wearable audio recorders and natural language processing technology, it’s become more practical to “listen” to hours of speech inside homes.

One pair of researchers, Kathleen Corriveau of Boston University and her former graduate student Katelyn Kurkul, now at Merrimack College, have produced a series of studies that delve into how some parents are answering their children’s questions and how those answers might make a difference in how we learn. The studies are small and haven’t been replicated but their work is an interesting glimpse into this new direction of early childhood research.

“Prior research has looked at frequency, how often parents and caregivers give explanations,” said Kurkul. “But there hasn’t been a lot of emphasis on how these explanations affect children’s learning.”

Corriveau and Kurkul’s most recent study of more than 100 four and five-year olds at Boston area preschools and a museum found that children who were exposed to more “mechanistic language” learned more than children who received shorter, less detailed explanations.

For example, if a child asks the question, “Why is there a battery?,” a simple, non-mechanistic answer could be, “To make it work.” That’s not wrong but it’s not very informative. A more detailed “mechanistic” response supplied by one parent in the study was: “The battery is there to give the toy power. When the batteries are connected to these buttons, then the toy will work because it has power.”

Measuring learning among young children is tricky. In this case, the researchers had children play with a snap circuit toy of many pieces that could be assembled to turn on a fan or a light bulb.

Children were just as likely to reassemble a fan system that was presented to them at the start of the exercise regardless of what type of explanation they received. But children who had heard the mechanistic type of explanation were twice as likely to assemble a new light bulb circuit that they hadn’t previously seen. That required them to understand the basics of a circuit and that the toy pieces had to be touching to turn the light on. The benefits of mechanistic explanations were true for both low- and middle-income children in the study.

“The mechanistic language is causing them to retain something about the concept that allows them to generalize it to a novel task,” said Kurkul.

In a previous study, the researchers noticed that children prefer non-circular answers to their questions. For example, a circular response to the question, “Why is the sky blue?” would be “Because it’s not another color.” When asked to choose between two adults, one who utters circular answers and one who doesn’t, kids tend to choose the person who offers the better non-circular reasoning, the researchers found. It didn’t matter if the answer was scientifically wrong; children seem to appreciate the attempt at a deeper explanation.

Related: Why talking — and listening — to your child could be key to brain development

Kurkul and Corriveau have compared the answers that low-income and middle-income caregivers typically offer children and they found stark differences. Low-income parents and caregivers were more likely to give circular answers to children’s questions than middle-class parents and caregivers. And now the researchers believe, from the latest snap circuit study, that the better answers are not just preferred but also leading to better learning.

With more cities offering early childhood education, Kurkul’s research is now shifting toward improving teacher quality in these classrooms.  She’s studying aspiring teachers to see what kinds of explanations they typically give and looking into how future teachers can be explicitly trained to avoid circular answers that don’t enlighten.

“It’s a challenge,” said Kurkul, “because you are bombarded with questions during these preschool years:  why, why why? But those questions are leading to learning. So the explanations you provide really do matter.”

This story about mechanistic explanations was written by Jill Barshay and produced by The Hechinger Report, a nonprofit, independent news organization focused on inequality and innovation in education. Sign up for the Hechinger newsletter.

The post How you talk to your child might make them smarter appeared first on The Hechinger Report.



from The Hechinger Report https://ift.tt/2n15IqZ

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Debacle over review reveals racism in academy (opinion)

When medievalist Mary Rambaran-Olm wrote about having her book review “torpedoed” for not being “more generous” to the book’s authors, no one could have expected that this would send shock waves across the academic community in what became an online maelstrom revealing the extent of white academic gatekeeping, ally performativity and blatant racism. For those of us who work on decentering whiteness in premodern fields such as classics, medieval/early modern studies, archaeology and in or on the Global South, this latest attack targeting a scholar of color exposed what many of us have been trying to draw attention to for years—that racism is deep and pernicious in the so-called liberal and woke academy. Rambaran-Olm was commissioned to review The Bright Ages: A New History of Medieval Europe ( HarperCollins ) for the Los Angeles Review of Books because of her expertise in early English medieval literature and history, and because she is one of the leading scholars challenging the

Consdierations for Another Uncertain Semester

Blog:  Just Visiting There are going to be a lot of sick people on college campuses in the fall. This is a pretty easy prediction because there are always a lot of sick people on college campuses given the very nature of the activities that happen on college campuses. I know I am not the only instructor to look out over a classroom and see lots of empty seats as students are felled by one virus or another.  I remember a particularly bad bout of mono that caught five students out of twenty in a single class and would’ve resulted in a passel of incompletes if I gave incompletes. (More on this in a moment.) While indications are that the coronavirus vaccines are holding up well against the Delta variant in preventing severe disease, hospitalization, and death, even vaccinated people are getting sick. It is beyond frustrating that a virus that could’ve been isolated and marginalized continues to thrive, but for now, as measured by the worst outcomes, we are collectively in a di